The recent discovery of three women in
Cleveland, Ohio, who had been abducted
for such an extended period, has
rekindled hopes that others long-missing
could still be found. The search for
Madeleine McCann appears to have been
re-invigorated, coinciding with the
recent publication of an
'age-progressed' photograph.
But new data from a recent series of
psychology experiments, investigating
how people recognize missing children,
are alarming. The results suggest that
the very techniques police forces around
the world are currently using, may
actually be making it harder to recover
missing children.
When a child has gone missing for an
extended period, predicting accurately
current appearance seems imperative.
This is currently accomplished via
forensic techniques known as 'age
progression,' in which an old photograph
of the missing person is used to predict
how the child would look now, using
computer modelling.
In the USA it is claimed that age
progression has helped to recover one of
out every seven children reported
missing to the National Center for
Missing and Exploited Children. In
almost every case in which age
progression is used, it's also claimed
new leads are generated.
Linked to the release of the
age-progressed image of Madeleine
McCann, the media have widely reported
that UK detectives reviewing the case of
her 2007 disappearance have identified
"a number of persons of interest".
Although increasingly widely used, and
offering much hope to distressed
relatives and searchers, whether the
technique actually aids recognition, has
not been properly scientifically tested.
But psychologists Steve Charman and
Rolando Carol, from Florida
International University, have recently
claimed in a new study, that
age-progressed images might even harm
recognition.
This has serious and profound
implications for the current search
strategy for Madeleine McCann, and
others, particularly given how much
publicity current age-progressed images
have received all around the world.
In this research, participants are
presented with either an outdated image
of a child, an age-progressed image of a
child, or both images, and then are
exposed to a series of faces of young
adults, and then asked to indicate
whether any of them are the 'target' or
missing child.
Charman and Carol found in their study
that the addition of an age-progressed
image significantly harmed recognition
of the child, and significantly inflated
false recognition.
The current study entitled
'Age-progressed images may harm
recognition of missing children by
increasing the number of plausible
targets' found that the age-progressed
images were not just simply decreasing
the likelihood of recognizing anyone,
but they seemed to be systematically
leading people away from recognizing the
target (and toward mistakenly
'recognizing' non-targets).
Charman and Carol's recent finding is
absolutely crucial to the field of
missing children investigation, as this
remains one of the only proper
investigations of this popular
technique, and it indicates
age-progressed images may actually harm
ability to recognize a target.
Charman and Carol acknowledge this
result is intriguing and
counterintuitive: If the age-progressed
image was a poor representation of the
target, participants who viewed both an
outdated and an age-progressed image
could have simply ignored it and relied
solely upon the outdated image. But they
clearly did not: In fact, they performed
worse than participants who viewed only
the outdated image.
The detrimental effect of age-progressed
images is most probably partly a
psychological effect: The addition of an
age-progressed image somehow changes
observers' decision-making strategies,
and does so in a profoundly unhelpful
way.
Charman and Carol conducted further
studies to investigate the precise
mechanism by which age-progressed images
seem to impede recognition of missing
children. Adding an age-progressed image
to an outdated image appears to
effectively create a second target face
that people use when looking for the
target. But the age-progressed image is
not a very accurate representation of
what the actual missing child currently
looks like. Therefore, the
age-progressed image increases the
number of competing non-target faces
that are seen as possibly being the
target.
Because more faces are now competing
with the target's face for recognition,
this results in lower recognition of the
missing child, and inflated mistaken
recognition of other faces.
Charman and Carol point out there are
two possible negative costs associated
with a recognition error produced by
age-progressed images in the real world:
An observer may mistakenly 'recognize' a
non-target (a false alarm) or may fail
to recognize an actual target (a miss).
But these errors are not equal: The
failure to recognize a missing child is
much more serious than mistakenly
'recognizing' someone.
Consequently, an age-progression
procedure that increased hits would be
beneficial, even if it led to an increase
in false alarms. The problem is that
these new results suggest that
age-progressed images seem to actually
reduce the likelihood of correctly
recognizing a missing child.
In other words, age-progressed images
were not simply useless; they were in
fact worse than useless, leading people
away from the actual 'missing child'.
If observers behaved logically, then
adding an age-progressed image to an
outdated image should lead them to
narrow in on a target. But, in contrast,
it actually increases the number of
plausible targets.
Basically people do not respond
logically to age-progressed images.
Their data, published in the 'Journal of
Applied Research in Memory and
Cognition' suggests that instead of
realizing that the target must be a
plausible match to both the outdated
image and the age-progressed image (or,
if the age-progressed image is perceived
to be completely worthless, to only the
outdated image), people seem to respond
to age-progressed images by reasoning
that the target must match either the
outdated image or the age-progressed
image, but not necessarily both.
Age-progression techniques are
problematic not only because the
algorithms of those techniques by which
the photo is generated could be flawed,
but also because observers are using
information derived from age-progressed
images incorrectly.
Charman and Carol conclude their study
by pointing out the anecdotal evidence
from The National Center for Missing and
Exploited Children, which claims: "In
virtually every case the production and
distribution of an updated [i.e.,
age-progressed] image stimulates new
leads" may not in fact be the good news
it is touted to be.
Any purported increase in leads may just
tend to be false recognitions of
non-targets. Given the recent much
trumpeted 'good news' suggesting the
possible generation of new leads over
Madeleine McCann, there is an ominous
possibility suggested by this new
research, that the hunt is heading in
the wrong direction. |